Skip to main content

Song divergence indicates an unclear relationship between the Neotropical and Nearctic Horned Larks

Abstract

Background

Some studies have indicated that the Horned Larks (Eremophila alpestris) should be considered as a species complex. Recently it split into four species or clades and seven subclades based on genetic, morphological, and biogeographic data. However, other aspects like song divergence have not been studied and several subspecies have not been evaluated, leading to important information gaps in this group of birds. In this work, we aimed to assess the differences in song traits and playback response between the Nearctic subclade and the Neotropical or Colombian subspecies E. a. peregrina.

Methods

We compared six song traits between these groups and performed field playback experiments, to test the response of the Neotropical larks to both songs. We tested the difference in the variables for separate as well as by principal component analysis (PCA).

Results

We found significant differences (pā€‰<ā€‰0.05) in the individual song traits and the PCA analysis between the two groups. Further, the PCA analysis showed a clearer divergence of the Neotropical songs in comparison to the Nearctic songs of different locations within North America. Similarly, the playback analysis showed a significantly lower response of E. a. peregrina to the songs of the Nearctic larks.

Conclusions

Besides this song divergence, there are important ecological and biogeographic differences between the Neotropical and Nearctic Horned Larks, that indicate an unclear relationship between these two groups. Thus, further morphological and genetic studies are required to clarify the taxonomy of the Neotropical Horned Lark and define if they share the same evolutionary history as the other subspecies of the Nearctic subclade.

Background

The Horned Larks (Eremophila alpestris) are a group of birds of wide distribution and more than 40 subspecies had been described (Drovetski et al. 2014). These larks represent an interesting model of study regarding the speciation process, as there are important morphological, biogeographic, and genetic differences between clades and they seem to have rapid phenotypic changes in response to habitat modification (Mason and Unitt 2018; Ghorbani et al. 2019).

In 2013, a taxonomic study on the Alaudidae (larks) family using molecular techniques indicated that the Horned Larks could be considered as a species complex (where speciation boundaries are unclear and potentially different species are grouping under the same scientific name) (Alstrƶm et al. 2013). Later in 2014, a molecular analysis with samples of different geographic locations of the Horned Larks reinforced the need to split these birds into multiple species and proposed its division in seven different species, including six Palearctic (elwesi, bilopha, penicillata, atlas, brandti, and flava lineages) and one Nearctic (alpestris lineage) clades (Drovetski et al. 2014). However, a research in 2019 proposed the division of the Horned Larks into only four different species or clades and seven subclades, composed of (1) the Common Horned Lark (E. alpestris), subdivided into the Nearctic and the North Palearctic subclades (brandti, and flava lineages); (2) the Mountain Horned Lark (E. penicillata), subdivides into the penicillata and the atlas subclades; (3) the Temminckā€™s Lark (E. bilopha); and (4) the Himalayan Horned Lark (E. longirostris), subdivided into the longirostris and elwesi subclades (Ghorbani et al. 2019).

Nevertheless, there is a lack of information on several subspecies of the Horned Lark, so the taxonomy of this group of birds is still uncertain. The great majority of the Horned Larks have located in the Holarctic region, and both the Nearctic and Palearctic populations present a wide distribution and a high number of subspecies (several with migratory patterns). Nonetheless, in the Neotropics, there is only one particular and sedentary subspecies (E. a. peregrina) that is restricted to the high plateau of the Colombian Eastern Andes (Beason 1995; Cadena 2002; Valencia and Armenteras 2004), and it is endangered (EN) due to the urbanization and displacement of the native grasses (Renjifo et al. 2016; Zuluaga-Bonilla and Macana-GarcĆ­a 2016). This bird is among the subspecies that have not been sampled in the genetic studies of the Horned Larks. So, there is no approximation of its speciation process or taxonomic status.

Also, bioacoustics divergence analyses have not been included in the speciation studies of the Horned Larks. SuchĀ information has proved to be useful in the analysis of speciation process and taxonomic revision of cryptic passerines (Toews and Irwin 2008). Song recognition is an important feature for birds evolutionary processes, as acoustic divergences preclude the conspecific recognition, causing barriers in gene flow, prevention of hybridization and finally promoting the speciation (Slabbekoorn and Smith 2002; Derryberry et al. 2018). Thus, analysis of song divergence and playback response has been important to study the taxonomic relation of birds (Alstrƶm and Ranft 2003). This approach has identified significant differences in song parameters and behavioral response to playback experiments among some subspecies of passerine birds, revealing significant premating reproductive isolation between populations (Dingle et al. 2010; Pieplow and Francis 2011; de Oliveira Gordinho et al. 2016; Slender et al. 2018). Further, evaluation of stimuli response in 72 pairs of closely related but geographically distant (allopatric) Neotropical passerines concluded that playback experiments are a suitable option in the study of the speciation process in birds with taxonomic uncertainty (Freeman and Montgomery 2017).

We aimed to evaluate the differences in songs and behavioral responses of the Neotropical Horned Lark compared to the Nearctic subclade to expand the knowledge on the evolutionary history of this diverse and complex group of birds. To achieve this, we evaluated potential differences in the songā€™s traits. Then, we tested if these variations in the songs could be biologically relevant by evaluating their effect on the behavioral response using playback experiments.

Methods

Biology of the Colombian Horned Lark and preliminary observational study

The study was done in La Copa reservoir, a montane ecosystem, located at an elevation of 2660Ā m in the central Andean mountains at the municipality of Toca in the BoyacĆ” state (Fig.Ā 1). We selected this area because it harbors the largest populations of the Neotropical or Colombian Horned Lark and has reports of nesting (Valencia and Armenteras 2004; BotĆ­a-Becerra and Echeverry-Galvis 2010). A previous study by our research group showed that at the beginning of the reproductive season, both males and females presented an intense vocal activity and there were some fights between adult males (Arias-Sosa et al. 2021). Later the larks were observed mainly in couples and both parents were involved in the care of the nest and offspring, indicating that this bird is at least seasonally monogamous (Arias-Sosa et al. 2021).

Fig. 1
figure 1

Study area with the recording and playback points

In North America, the Horned Larks tend to define territories during the reproductive season that are defended by the males (Beason and Franks 1974; Cannings and Threlfall 1981; DeGraaf and Yamasaki 2001). Nevertheless, in our study area, there were fights between adult males but they didnā€™t define territories during the mating season; because the area was small and the population density was high, the larks were hinderedĀ to have independent well-defined territories (Arias-Sosa et al. 2021).

As we aimed to make playback experiments, we implemented a preliminary test with some recordings of males and females obtained during field observations. In this test, we realized that both males and females responded to the playback similarly, emitting responding songs and getting closer to the speaker. We distinguished males and females based on the distinctive facial marks of the adult individuals. About 60% of responses were from females and 40% from males. The majority of playback experiments only take into account male response, based on the assumption that the female response (mate choice) would follow the same pattern, but this may not be true in all cases (Matessi et al. 2000; Dingle et al. 2010; Pegan et al. 2015; de Oliveira Gordinho et al. 2016). Considering the female response is particularly important in the tropical region whereĀ it is more common that female birds present a high song activity and response to playback stimuli (Slater and Mann 2004; Illes and Yunes-Jimenez 2009), we thereforeĀ decided to include both sexes in the experiments.

Also, during the playback preliminary test, we realized that multiple individuals responded during the same experiment (as the Horned Larks in the zone presented a gregarious behavior). It was difficult to differentiate the individual response due to the small size of the birds and because they usually move fast, which makes challenging to distinguish which individual of the group was emitting the response singing. Thus, we decided to implement the playback with all the birds in a radius of 25Ā m per experiment rather than individual analysis.

During the preliminary experiments, we also observed the time it takes for the birds to return to their normal behavior after the acoustic stimuli. We observed that after 1Ā min the birds returned to look for food on the ground and move away from the speaker. Thus, we established that 2Ā min of silence between experiments would be adequate to avoid interference between them.

As we mentioned previously during the preliminary studies, we could recognize the sex of several birds using binoculars at a close to medium distance (2ā€’10Ā m) and by following a single individual. However, when we tried to measure the playback response at a far distance (30Ā m, required for the playback experiments), it was difficult to accurately recognize the sex of each bird. Because the identification of the sexual dimorphism in this species requires great attention to particular marks on the face and chest, we thusĀ could not perform a separate sex analysis.

Acoustical traits analyses

Recording collection

The songs of the Neotropical Horned Lark were obtained by field recording in La Copa Reservoir from November 2017 to May 2018. The recordings were obtained from natural vocalizations using a unidirectional microphone (Takstar SGC 568) and were stored in a digital device (Tascam DR 07 MK II MDR12). The same device was used for all recordings, and files were converted to WAV 16-bit, 44.1Ā kHz. To avoid pseudoreplication we took the recordings in line transect across the 6 points studied (Fig.Ā 1). We took recordings of one singing lark, and then we moved on to take a record of another individual. Overall, we got 40 recordings from E. a. peregrina.

The most recent work in the taxonomy of the Horned Larks proposed it split into four different species or clades, three of them distributed in the western Palearctic and one distributed in the North Palearctic and the Nearctic (Ghorbani et al. 2019). This last species of the Horned Lark was named E. alpestris (sensu stricto) and is subdivided into two subclades: the Nearctic and the North Palearctic subclade. We chose to compare the songs of E. a. peregrina with the Nearctic subclade because it is the geographically closest one and the one with more recordings available. Also because the previous genetics studies indicated a single colonization process of the New-world and a common evolutionary history of the New-world larks (although they only considered some North American populations) (Ghorbani et al. 2019).

We seached for recordings of the Nearctic E. alpestris in the Macaulay Library (Cornell Lab of Ornithology 2018; https://www.macaulaylibrary.org/) and Xeno-canto (Xeno-canto Foundation 2018; https://www.xeno-canto.org/). We had into account only recordings longer than 30Ā s. These files were obtained in MP3 format and converted to WAV 16-bit, 44.1Ā kHz. We retrieved 88 recordings.

Song analysis

All recordings were digitized and analyzed using AvisoftSAS Lab Lite software (Sound Analysis and Synthesis Laboratory Version 5.2.12.01 December 2017) (TurčokovĆ” et al. 2010; Hamao et al. 2013). From an initial analysis of the recordings, we found two types of vocalizations of the Horned Lark, a short call composed of two or three notes with a length of 0.5Ā s, and another with a mean of 10 notes and a length of about 2Ā s. This last one was considered the principal song and was the only one used in the next analysis. We selected this song because it presented more notes and traits that allow better analysis. We observed that the short call is used to communicate between close individuals, especially between the parents with their offspring.

To eliminate external interferences like wind, songs of other birds, and human noise we applied the infinite impulse response (IIR) and finite impulse response (FIR) filters to each recording. These are common and effective mathematical algorithms used in digital acoustic management and in birdsā€™ bioacoustics studies to reduce noise and avoid artifacts (Leonardo 2004; Mockford and Marshall 2009; Rimell et al. 2015). We selected the adequate cut-off frequencies for each song based on the sonogram as it varied between recordings (Leonardo 2004; Mockford and Marshall 2009).

From the original 40 Colombian and 88 Nearctic recordings retrieved, we excluded 20 and 60 recordings respectively because they only contained short calls, had incomplete songs, presented too much background noise, or had a bad sound quality. Thus, we continued the analysis with only 20 Colombian and 28 Nearctic recordings (Additional file 1: Table S1).

For each recording, we extrapolated the songs and excluded those that presented significant noise based on the clarity of the spectrogram. From this analysis, we got 71 songs (obtained from the 20 recordings) of the Colombian lark and 78 songs (obtained from the 28 recordings) of the Nearctic larks. We analyzed these songs using the software AvisoftSAS (with a ā€“ā€‰5 to ā€“ā€‰10Ā dB threshold) to get the number of notes, the number of notes per second, song length, the time between notes (average), the highest frequency, and the lowest frequency. We selected these song features because they have been described as parameters of divergence between passerines (Dingle et al. 2008, 2010).

Data analysis

All statistical analyses were performed using the free software R (3.6 version) and the package R Commander (2.5ā€“3 version), with a pā€‰<ā€‰0.05 as significant (R Core Team 2020). We obtained several songs from the same recording (and potentially the same individual) and we observed interesting variation between them. Thus, we made two separate analyses: (1) using all the obtained songs to get an analysis that included the individual variation and (2) using one randomly selected song per recording to get an analysis not affected by individual pseudoreplication.

As the variables did not present normal distribution (evaluated by Shapiroā€“Wilk and Kolmogorovā€“Smirnov tests), the differences in acoustic features were analyzed by the nonparametric Mannā€“Whitney U test (TurčokovĆ” et al. 2010). Later we implemented a principal component analysis (PCA), using Past software (Hammer et al. 2001) to combine the acoustic variables into a single one named ā€œsong divergenceā€. It corresponded to the first axis of the PCA (PC1) that presented an eigenvalueā€‰>ā€‰1 and was evaluated by the ANOVA test as it was normally distributed. The PCA was done because there was a probable correlation between the variables based on Bartlettā€™s Test of Sphericity (BTS) and Kaiser-Meyer Olkin test (KMO) in both analyses: using all songs (BTS pā€‰<ā€‰001, KMOā€‰=ā€‰0.48) and one song per recording (BTS pā€‰<ā€‰001, KMOā€‰=ā€‰0.46).

Finally, we plot the PC1 vs PC2 using a scatterplot to see the grouping of the Nearctic and Neotropical songs. As the Nearctic songs come from different locations in North America, we show the locations in the scatterplot classified in: Central-West (CW), North-East (NE), North-West (NW), South-West (SW), and South USA-Mexico (SM). This classification was based on the subdivision of the Nearctic Horned Larks proposed by Ghorbani et al. (2019) (except South USA-Mexico that was not included in this previous study).

Playback experiment

General description of the experiments

The playback experiments evaluated the degree of recognition and response strength by individuals of E. a. peregrina exposed to the conspecific and Neotropical songs stimuli. The experiments were carried out in the northern part of La Copa Reservoir, where our research group registered the highest densities of E. a. peregrina and several nests (Arias-Sosa et al. 2021).

In each playback experiment, we tested the response to three stimuli: (1) songs of the conspecific populations (Neotropical), (2) songs of the allopatric populations (Nearctic), and (3) response to a different species (Rufous-collared Sparrow Zonotrichia capensis) as a control group. Z. capensis is a common bird of the Colombian Andes that cohabitates naturally in sympatry with the lark and present similar size and behavior (feeding on seeds and insects in grasslands). The songs from the Rufous-collared Sparrow were obtained from Xeno-canto recordings from BoyacĆ”, Colombia. In the conspecific (Neotropical) stimuli, we used songs from other localities (the farthest away in the southern region of the reservoir) to avoid the familiarity of the larks with the local dialect.

Preparation of the experiments

We implemented a total of 36 playback experiments. Each experiment consisted of the broadcasting of a recording, composed of 2Ā min mix of songs of each one of the three stimuli (Neotropical, Nearctic, and control) separated by 2Ā min of silence between each other. We prepared each recording with a different mix of songs to avoid potential bias (Pegan et al. 2015). To prepare each recording we had a set of 38 songs of each of the three stimuli. We randomly selected 10 songs per stimuli, added a random pause between songs, and randomly repeated them until complete the 2Ā min stimuli. All recordings were normalized in the range of 30ā€“80Ā dB.

Field conditions of the experiments

We broadcast the recordings using an iPod Touch 4G 8Ā GB player through amplification with an EXTRA BASSā„¢ XB32 portable speaker at 40ā€“80Ā dB at 1Ā m, placed on the ground (because the Horned Larks stay most of the time foraging on the floor). We started the experiments only if the close birds were looking for food and not singing by themselves (indicators of a non-stimulated state), to reduce the risk of getting an intrinsic response (not related to the playback stimuli) (de Oliveira Gordinho et al. 2016; Slender et al. 2018).

To reduce the effect of pseudoreplication the experiments were performed in different locations and with one-hour intervals to avoid interference between each other. To avoid potential bias the conspecific (local population) songs were always reproduced after the allopatric and control songs (Sagario and Cueto 2014). This because the conspecific song could overstimulate the response of the birds in the subsequent stimulus (Sagario and Cueto 2014). In the case of Nearctic and control treatment, we alternated its order. The response to the conspecific songs is unlikely to be affected by the reproduction order because as we observe in the preliminary observational study 2Ā min of silence is enough for the birds to return to their normal (unstimulated) behavior. During all the playback experiments we confirm that the target birds returned to their feeding behavior and get away from the speaker within the 2Ā min of silence.

Data collection

Once we started the experiment, we registered the behavioral responses at approximately 30Ā m from the speaker using binoculars. We followed the response during the 2Ā min of each stimulus and the subsequent 1Ā min of silence. As the Horned Larks in our study area presented a gregarious behavior that makes it difficult to take individual data, we retrieved the information of all responding larks per experiment. As we reproduce the 3 archives in a consecutive way the same number of individuals were exposed to each stimulus. In some of the experiments, we could observe that both sexes sang in response and approached the speaker; however, we could not differentiate the sex of most of the individuals and we were not able of making a sex-specific analysis. We evaluated the following response variables:

  1. 1.

    The approach response: the number of birds that approached the speaker. This variable was subdivided into: approach within a 20-m radius and an approach within a 10-m radius. A higher number of larks approaching indicates a stronger response.

  2. 2.

    The response vocalization: the number of songs emitted by the larks during the experiments on a radius of 25Ā m. A higher number of response songs indicates a stronger response.

  3. 3.

    The latency to approach: the time (in seconds) that it took for the first birds to approached the speaker within a 5-m radius. When no bird approached, we reported the measure as 180Ā s (the 3Ā min of observation). A lower time is associated with a stronger response.

These variables were selected because they have been described as consistent indicators of behavioral response to playback in passerine birds (Matessi et al. 2001; Jankowski et al. 2010; de Oliveira Gordinho et al. 2016; Freeman et al. 2016; Slender et al. 2018).

The research was done under a research project made in collaboration with the ā€œCorporaciĆ³n AutĆ³noma Regional de BoyacĆ”ā€ the governmental entity that regulates environmental activities in the state. None individual was captured, manipulated, or suffered any damage.

Data analysis

The differences in the playback variables were evaluated by the Kruskalā€“Wallis test with a Bonferroni post-hoc test. Because there is a probable correlation between the variables (BTS pā€‰<ā€‰001, KMOā€‰=ā€‰0.7), we used principal component analysis (PCA), using Past software to combinate the playback variables into a single one named ā€œresponse intensityā€. This variable corresponded to the first axis of the PCA (PC1) with an eigenvalueā€‰>ā€‰1, and was evaluated by the ANOVA test with a Bonferroni post-hoc test as it was normally distributed. Also, we used a PC1 vs PC2 scatterplot to observe the difference in the response to the three stimuli.

Results

Acoustical traits analyses

The songs of the Neotropical larks presented some general and notorious differences from those of the Nearctic populations when we observed the spectrograms (Additional file 2: Figure S1). The Neotropical songs were generally paused (with a longer pause between notes), shorter, and with a lower number of notes. While the Neotropical songs were longer, with more notes, and with a lower pause between notes.

Both analyses (using all the retrieved songs and one song per recording) showed a significant difference in the number of notes, song length, the time between notes, highest frequency, and lowest frequency (Table 1; Fig.Ā 2). On the other hand, the number of notes per second was significantly different in the analysis using all the retrieved songs (pā€‰<ā€‰0.001) but evidenced a notorious overlapping between the two groups (Fig.Ā 2) and it was not significantly different when we take only one song per recording (pā€‰=ā€‰0.36). Thus, there seem to be differences in the six song traits. But the clearest divergent ones were the number of notes, the time between notes, and the frequency limits. Regarding these variables, the songs from the Neotropical Horned Lark presented a lower number of notes, a higher time between notes (longer pauses), and higher frequencies than the Nearctic songs (Table 1).

Table 1 Differences in Song features
Fig. 2
figure 2

Differences in the acoustic traits among the Nearctic and Neotropical Horned Larks. We showed a boxplot of each variable evaluated in: a the analysis using all retrieved songs, and b the analysis using one song per recording.*ā€‰=ā€‰pā€‰<ā€‰0.05; **ā€‰=ā€‰pā€‰<ā€‰0.005; ***ā€‰=ā€‰pā€‰<ā€‰0.001

For the PCA analysis, we found a significant difference in the PC1 component (named song divergence) between the Nearctic and Neotropical lark songs in both analyses (Fig.Ā 3). Likewise, the scatterplot showed a clear differentiation between the Nearctic and Neotropical songs (Fig.Ā 3). While, there was high overlap in the songs of the different geographic subdivisions of the Nearctic clade. This indicates that the Neotropical lark presents more divergent song traits than the observed within the different populations in North America (that have a recent genetic divergence). The loads and eigenvalues of the PCA are shown in Additional file 1: Tables S2ā€“4.

Fig. 3
figure 3

Boxplot of the song divergence (PC1) between the Nearctic and Neotropical songs and PC1 vs PC2 scatterplot evaluated in: a the analysis using all retrieved songs, and b the analysis using one song per recording. The ellipses of the scatterplot were calculated automatically in the R software using the option of concentration ellipses (.5.8Ā°). For the Nearctic songs, we show its geographic subclassification: Central-West (CW), North-East (NE), North-West (NW), South-West (SW), and South USA-Mexico (SM). ***ā€‰=ā€‰pā€‰<ā€‰0.001

Playback response

We still observed an important degree of approach response to the Nearctic stimuli but it was significantly lower (less lark approached) than the response to the conspecific songs within 25Ā m (pā€‰<ā€‰0.05) and 10Ā m radius (pā€‰<ā€‰0.001) (Fig.Ā 4). Likewise, the larks exposed to the conspecific (Neotropical) stimuli emitted more reply songs and approached faster (pā€‰<ā€‰0.001) (Table 2). Compared to the experiments with the North American stimuli, there were about three times more larks approached to the 10Ā m radius in response to the conspecific song stimuli (3.8ā€‰Ā±ā€‰1.6 larks vs 1.3ā€‰Ā±ā€‰1.1 larks), they approached two times faster to a 5Ā m radius to the speaker (63.2ā€‰Ā±ā€‰27.7Ā s vs 121.3ā€‰Ā±ā€‰33.4Ā s) and emitted three times more reply songs (9.5ā€‰Ā±ā€‰2.9 songs vs 2.8ā€‰Ā±ā€‰1.4 songs). On the other hand, the response to the control experiment was minimal as expected (Fig.Ā 4). This indicates that although the Colombian Horned Lark seems to respond to the Nearctic songs, the strength of this response is notoriously lower.

Fig. 4
figure 4

Differences in the playback response to the Nearctic, Neotropical, and control (Rufous-collared Sparrow) stimuli. a Boxplot of each variable evaluated. b Boxplot of the response intensity variable (PC1). Lower PC1 values indicate a stronger response as it reflects a faster and closer approach with more reply vocalizations. Also, we show the PC1 vs PC2 scatterplot. The ellipses of the scatter plot were calculated automatically in the R software using the option of concentration ellipses (.5.8 degree). **ā€‰=ā€‰pā€‰<ā€‰0.005; ***ā€‰=ā€‰pā€‰<ā€‰0.001

Table 2 Differences in the response to playback experiments

The PCA analysis showed a significant difference in the PC1 (response intensity) between the three groups (pā€‰<ā€‰0.001) and the scatterplot evidenced a notable separation between the response to the Nearctic and Neotropical stimuli (Fig.Ā 4). The loads and eigenvalues of the PCA are shown in Additional file 1: Table S2ā€“4.

Discussion

This is the first study that evaluated differences in songs of allopatric populations of E. alpestris, and the first speciation work in E. a. peregrina. The Colombian subspecies presented differences in the song traits and PCA analysis indicating some modifications in the dialect compared to the Nearctic subclade. Also, we evidence differences in behavioral response during playback experiments. The Colombian Horned Lark seems to respond to the song of the Nearctic larks but with a lower intensity than to local vocalizations.

The differences found indicate that male Nearctic Horned Larks may be perceived as the least significant sexual competitors by the Colombian subspecies, and females prefer the local songs which could lead to a premating reproductive isolation (Searcy 1992; Ballentine et al. 2004; Balakrishnan and Sorenson 2006). However, as we could not make a distinction between male and female responses, further sex-specific studies are required to explore if the preference for local songs occurs in the same degree between males and females. The difference in the response between local and Nearctic songs occurs because birds learn their songs from neighboring conspecifics and distant populations tend to lost similarity, especially in sedentary species with breeding site fidelity (like E. a. peregrina) (Morton 1987; Kroodsma 2002).

The combination of vocal traits analysis and playback experiments has been described as a robust measure of reproductive isolation (Pegan et al. 2015; Freeman and Montgomery 2017). This approach has been used as part of the criteria for the delimitation of species limits and in speciation studies, and joining the mitochondrial DNA analysis, are among the most divergent characteristics between bird species (Alstrƶm and Olsson 1992; Alstrƶm et al. 2008). Some examples of this use are the analysis of subspecies of White-breasted (Henicorhina leucosticte) and Gray-breasted Wood-Wren (H. leucophrys) that presented differences in song traits and an asymmetric playback response, described as a premating reproductive barrier that suggested a complex of multiple biological species (Dingle et al. 2010; Pegan et al. 2015). Research in recently separated subspecies of Reed Bunting (Emberiza schoeniclus) showed differences in song traits, but less clear differences in playback response, indicating that this population experiences an incipient or early stage of speciation (Matessi et al. 2000; Gordinho et al. 2015; de Oliveira Gordinho et al. 2016). While in subspecies of the Buff-breasted Earthcreeper (Upucerthia validirostris) no differences in playback response were found and the researchers recommended keep them as the same species (Areta and Pearman 2013). However other important features like DNA, morphometrics, plumage traits, biometrics, egg characteristics, and biogeography need to be considered in the species delimitation (Alstrƶm and Olsson 1992; Alstrƶm et al. 2008).

In our case, we found that the Neotropical larks still respond to the Nearctic songs but in a significantly lower degree, and there are important differences in the song traits between these two groups. Further, the Neotropical lark songs are more clearly differentiated than the subgroups of Horned Lark in the Nearctic (Fig.Ā 3). The fact that the Neotropical larks still respond to the Nearctic songs is consistent with the conclusion of previous studies that indicate that the New-world Horned Larks subspecies are recently divergent between each other and are part of the same species or clade (Ghorbani et al. 2019). However, the high degree of differentiation in the songs and lower response to playback seem to indicate a particular evolutionary history of the Neotropical Horned Larks.

Besides the song differences that we found, the Neotropical lark presents other important ecological and biogeographic differences in comparison to the Nearctic populations and the North Palearctic larks that compose the alpestris clade. E. a peregrina is highly isolated (similar to an island type isolation) from other Horned Larks groups, have a very restricted distribution area (160 km2), and only inhabited the high Plateau of the Eastern Andes between the 2500ā€’3200Ā m a.s.l. (Valencia and Armenteras 2004). While, the larks in North America and the North Palearctic have a wide distribution (of most of the continent) and exploit different habitats that go from the coastā€™s region to the high mountains. Also, the clutch size of the Neotropical Horned Lark (two eggs) is lower than the observed in the Nearctic populations (2ā€’5 eggs) (Beason and Franks 1974; Cannings and Threlfall 1981; Garry Oak Ecosystems Recovery Team 2003; Camfield et al. 2010). Further, these characteristics are more similar to the clades/species in the South Palearctic that present an isolated and reduced distribution, associated with mountain habitats. Like the Himalayan Horned Lark clade (E. longirostris), which presents a restricted distribution area in the high Qinghaiā€“Tibetan Plateau and clutch size of 1ā€’3 eggs (Ghorbani et al. 2019). Unfortunately, there are too few recordings of this Horned Lark clade to compare with the Neotropical songs.

Thus, the Neotropical Horned Lark (E. a. peregrina) has important bioacoustic, biogeographic, and ecological differences compared to the Nearctic subclade that makes unclear its evolutionary relation. Genetic and morphological comparison of the Neotropical lark with the established clades of the Horned Lark is required to assess its taxonomic status and evolutionary history. With the current information, we can propose two hypotheses: (1) the Neotropical subspecies follows the evolutionary history observed by Ghorbani et al. (2019) and Drovetski et al. (2014), who established that the New-world Horned Larks subspecies come from a single colonization event from the Palearctic and have a common evolutionary history (monophyly); (2) the Neotropical Horned Lark has a separate evolutionary history from the Nearctic clade and could be originated from a different colonization event. However, these hypotheses need to be evaluated in future studies using DNA sampling.

One limitation of our study was that we did not evaluate the reciprocal responses in the Nearctic populations. So, we suggest that future researches focus on the response of Nearctic Horned Larks to the vocalizations of Neotropical birds. Also, it would be interesting to analyze the song traits of the other three clades of Horned Larks in the south Palearctic and make playback experiments using these songs to have more clear information of the song divergence in contrast to the known genetic variance (Drovetski et al. 2014; Ghorbani et al. 2019). Also, we were incapable of recognizing the sex of the tested larks in the field experiments, which implies a lack of information on the specific response of male and female larks. Although in the preliminary observational study, we realized that both males and females respond to playback experiments, some research had found that the behavioral response of birds exposed to acoustic signals can be contrasting between males and females (Snijders et al. 2017; Bircher et al. 2020). Likewise, the response of the males could be affected by the presence of a female partner around. Thus, further studies are necessary to determine the exact degree of song differentiation by males and females of the Horned Larks.

Conclusions

We evidenced important differences in the song traits between the Nearctic and Neotropical Horned Larks. Further, the song divergence of the Neotropical larks was more pronounced than those found within the Nearctic subgroups. In the playback analysis, we found that the Neotropical lark still presents an important degree of recognition to the Nearctic songs, but the strength of the response was significantly lower. Besides this acoustic difference, the neotropical Horned Larks display particular biogeographic, behavioral, and reproductive traits that are more related to some clades in the South Palearctic. Thus, the relation between the Nearctic and Neotropical larks is uncertain and further genetic studies are required to establish if the Neotropical larks had the same colonization and dispersion history described in the New World subspecies of North America or experienced a separate evolutionary history.

Availability of data and materials

The datasets used in the present study are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.

References

  • Alstrƶm P, Olsson U. On the taxonomic status of Phylloscopus affinis and Phylloscopus subaffinis. Bull Br Ornithol Club. 1992;112:112ā€“25.

    Google ScholarĀ 

  • Alstrƶm P, Ranft R. The use of sounds in avian systematics and the importance of bird sound archives. Bull Ornithol Club. 2003;123A:114ā€“35.

    Google ScholarĀ 

  • Alstrƶm P, Rasmussen PC, Olsson U, Sundberg P. Species delimitation based on multiple criteria: the Spotted Bush Warbler Bradypterus thoracicus complex (Aves: Megaluridae). Zool J Linn Soc. 2008;154:291ā€“307.

    ArticleĀ  Google ScholarĀ 

  • Alstrƶm P, Barnes KN, Olsson U, Barker FK, Bloomer P, Khan AA, et al. Multilocus phylogeny of the avian family Alaudidae (larks) reveals complex morphological evolution, non-monophyletic genera and hidden species diversity. Mol Phylogenet Evol. 2013;69:1043ā€“56.

    ArticleĀ  PubMedĀ  Google ScholarĀ 

  • Areta JI, Pearman M. Species limits and clinal variation in a widespread high Andean Furnariid: the Buff-breasted Earthcreeper (Upucerthia validirostris). Condor. 2013;115:131ā€“42.

    ArticleĀ  Google ScholarĀ 

  • Arias-Sosa LA, Vargas-Puentes JA, Ramos-MontaƱo C, Espinosa-Blanco AS. Population status, habitat use and breeding biology of the endangered Colombian Horned Lark in an Andean wetland. Ardea. 2021;109:5ā€“14.

    ArticleĀ  Google ScholarĀ 

  • Balakrishnan CN, Sorenson MD. Song discrimination suggests premating isolation among sympatric indigobird species and host races. Behav Ecol. 2006;17:473ā€“8.

    ArticleĀ  Google ScholarĀ 

  • Ballentine B, Hyman J, Nowicki S. Vocal performance influences female response to male bird song: an experimental test. Behav Ecol. 2004;15:163ā€“8.

    ArticleĀ  Google ScholarĀ 

  • Beason RC. Horned Lark (Eremophila alpestris). In: Poole A, Gill F, editors. The birds of North America, No. 195. Washington, D. C.: The Academy of Natural Sciences, Philadelphia and the American Ornithologistsā€™ Union; 1995. p. 24.

    Google ScholarĀ 

  • Beason RC, Franks EC. Breeding behavior of the Horned Lark. Auk. 1974;91:65ā€“74.

    ArticleĀ  Google ScholarĀ 

  • Bircher N, van Oers K, Hinde CA, Naguib M. Extraterritorial forays by great tits are associated with dawn song in unexpected ways. Behav Ecol. 2020;31:873ā€“83.

    ArticleĀ  PubMedĀ  PubMed CentralĀ  Google ScholarĀ 

  • BotĆ­a-Becerra J, Echeverry-Galvis MƁ. First nesting records of the Horned Lark (Eremophila alpestris peregrina) in Colombia. Ornitol Colomb. 2010;10:55ā€“60.

    Google ScholarĀ 

  • Cadena CD. Eremophila alpestris. In: Renjifo L, Franco A, Amaya-Espinel J, Kattan G, LĆ³pez-Lanus, editors. Libro rojo aves Colombia Serie Libro Rojos Especies Amenazadas Colomb. BogotĆ”: Instituto de InvestigaciĆ³n de Recursos BiolĆ³gicos Alexander von Humboldt y Ministerio del Medio Ambiente; 2002. p. 371ā€“4.

    Google ScholarĀ 

  • Camfield AF, Pearson SF, Martin K. Life history variation between high and low elevation subspecies of horned larks Eremophila spp. J Avian Biol. 2010;41:273ā€“81.

    ArticleĀ  Google ScholarĀ 

  • Cannings RJ, Threlfall W. Horned lark breeding biology at Cape St. Maryā€™s. Newfoundland Wilson Bull. 1981;93:519ā€“30.

    Google ScholarĀ 

  • de Gordinho LO, Matheu E, Hasselquist D, Neto JM. Song divergence between subspecies of reed bunting is more pronounced in singing styles under sexual selection. Anim Behav. 2015;107:221ā€“31.

    ArticleĀ  Google ScholarĀ 

  • de Oliveira GL, Hasselquist D, Neto JM. Asymmetric song recognition between recently diverged subspecies of reed bunting. Behav Ecol. 2016;27:1413ā€“23.

    ArticleĀ  Google ScholarĀ 

  • DeGraaf RM, Yamasaki M. New England wildlife: habitat, natural history, and distribution. New Hampshire: University Press of New England; 2001.

    Google ScholarĀ 

  • Derryberry EP, Seddon N, Derryberry GE, Claramunt S, Seeholzer GF, Brumfield RT, et al. Ecological drivers of song evolution in birds: disentangling the effects of habitat and morphology. Ecol Evol. 2018;8:1890ā€“905.

    ArticleĀ  PubMedĀ  PubMed CentralĀ  Google ScholarĀ 

  • Dingle C, Halfwerk W, Slabbekoorn H. Habitat-dependent song divergence at subspecies level in the grey-breasted wood-wren. J Evol Biol. 2008;21:1079ā€“89.

    ArticleĀ  CASĀ  PubMedĀ  Google ScholarĀ 

  • Dingle C, Poelstra JW, Halfwerk W, Brinkhuizen DM, Slabbekoorn H. Asymmetric response patterns to subspecies-specific song differences in allopatry and parapatry in the gray-breasted wood-wren. Evolution (N Y). 2010;64:3537ā€“48.

    Google ScholarĀ 

  • Drovetski SV, Raković M, Semenov G, Fadeev IV, Redkin YA. Limited phylogeographic signal in sex-linked and autosomal loci despite geographically, ecologically, and phenotypically concordant structure of mtDNA variation in the Holarctic avian genus Eremophila. PLoS ONE. 2014;9:e87570.

    ArticleĀ  PubMedĀ  PubMed CentralĀ  CASĀ  Google ScholarĀ 

  • Freeman BG, Montgomery GA. Using song playback experiments to measure species recognition between geographically isolated populations: a comparison with acoustic trait analyses. Auk. 2017;134:857ā€“70.

    ArticleĀ  Google ScholarĀ 

  • Freeman BG, Class Freeman AM, Hochachka WM. Asymmetric interspecific aggression in New Guinean songbirds that replace one another along an elevational gradient. Ibis. 2016;158:726ā€“37.

    ArticleĀ  Google ScholarĀ 

  • Garry Oak Ecosystems Recovery Team. Eremophila alpestris strigata. Species at risk in Garry Oak and associated ecosystems in British Columbia. 1st ed. Victoria: Garry Oak Ecosystems Recovery Team; 2003.

    Google ScholarĀ 

  • Ghorbani F, Aliabadian M, Olsson U, Donald PF, Khan AA, Alstrƶm P. Mitochondrial phylogeography of the genus Eremophila confirms underestimated species diversity in the Palearctic. J Ornithol. 2019;161:297ā€“312.

    ArticleĀ  Google ScholarĀ 

  • Hamao S, Sugita N, Nishiumi I. Geographical variation in mitochondrial DNA and vocalizations in two resident bird species in the Ryukyu Archipelago. Jpn Bull Natl Mus Nat Sci-A. 2013;39:51ā€“62.

    Google ScholarĀ 

  • Hammer Ƙ, Harper DAT, Ryan PD. Past: paleontological statistics software package for education and data analysis. Palaeontol Electron. 2001;4:99.

    Google ScholarĀ 

  • Illes AE, Yunes-Jimenez L. A female songbird out-sings male conspecifics during simulated territorial intrusions. Proc R Soc B. 2009;276:981ā€“6.

    ArticleĀ  PubMedĀ  Google ScholarĀ 

  • Jankowski JE, Robinson SK, Levey DJ. Squeezed at the top: interspecific aggression may constrain elevational ranges in tropical birds. Ecology. 2010;91:1877ā€“84.

    ArticleĀ  PubMedĀ  Google ScholarĀ 

  • Kroodsma DE. Falkland Island Sedge Wrens (Cistothorus platensis) imitate rather than improvise large song repertoires. Auk. 2002;119:523ā€“8.

    ArticleĀ  Google ScholarĀ 

  • Leonardo A. Experimental test of the birdsong error-correction model. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2004;101:16935ā€“40.

    ArticleĀ  CASĀ  PubMedĀ  Google ScholarĀ 

  • Mason NA, Unitt P. Rapid phenotypic change in a native bird population following conversion of the Colorado Desert to agriculture. J Avian Biol. 2018;49:e01507.

    ArticleĀ  Google ScholarĀ 

  • Matessi G, Dabelsteen T, Pilastro A. Responses to playback of different subspecies songs in the Reed Bunting Emberiza schoeniclus. J Avian Biol. 2000;31:96ā€“101.

    ArticleĀ  Google ScholarĀ 

  • Matessi G, Dabelsteen T, Pilastro A. Subspecies song discrimination in a Mediterranean population of the reed bunting Emberiza schoeniclus intermedia. Ital J Zool. 2001;68:311ā€“4.

    ArticleĀ  Google ScholarĀ 

  • Mockford EJ, Marshall RC. Effects of urban noise on song and response behaviour in great tits. Proc R Soc B Biol Sci. 2009;276:2979ā€“85.

    ArticleĀ  Google ScholarĀ 

  • Morton ES. The effects of distance and isolation on song-type sharing in the Carolina Wren. Wilson Bull. 1987;99:601ā€“10.

    Google ScholarĀ 

  • Pegan TM, Rumelt RB, Dzielski SA, Ferraro MM, Flesher LE, Young N, et al. Asymmetric response of Costa Rican White-breasted Wood-Wrens (Henicorhina leucosticta) to vocalizations from allopatric populations. PLoS ONE. 2015;10:e0144949.

    ArticleĀ  PubMedĀ  PubMed CentralĀ  CASĀ  Google ScholarĀ 

  • Pieplow ND, Francis CD. Song differences among subspecies of Yellow-eyed Juncos (Junco phaeonotus). Wilson J Ornithol. 2011;123:464ā€“71.

    ArticleĀ  Google ScholarĀ 

  • R Core Team. R: a language and environment for statistical computing; 2020. https://www.r-project.org/.

  • Rimell AN, Mansfield NJ, Paddan GS. Design of digital filters for frequency weightings (A and C) required for risk assessments of workers exposed to noise. Ind Health. 2015;53:21ā€“7.

    ArticleĀ  PubMedĀ  Google ScholarĀ 

  • Renjifo LM, Amaya-Villarreal AM, Burbano-GirĆ³n J, VelĆ”squez-TibatĆ” J. Libro rojo de aves de Colombia, Volumen II: Ecosistemas abiertos, secos, insulares, acuĆ”ticos continentales, marinos, tierras altas del DariĆ©n y Sierra Nevada de Santa Marta y bosques hĆŗmedos del centro, norte y oriente del paĆ­s. BogotĆ”, D. C., Colombia: Editorial Pontificia Universidad Javeriana e Instituto Alexander von Humboldt; 2016.

    Google ScholarĀ 

  • Sagario MC, Cueto VR. EvaluaciĆ³n del comportamiento territorial de cuatro especies de aves granĆ­voras en el Monte Central. Hornero. 2014;29:81ā€“92.

    Google ScholarĀ 

  • Searcy WA. Measuring responses of female birds to male song. In: McGregor PK, editor. Playback and studies animal communication. Boston: Springer; 1992. p. 175ā€“89.

    ChapterĀ  Google ScholarĀ 

  • Slabbekoorn H, Smith TB. Bird song, ecology and speciation. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 2002;357:493ā€“503.

    ArticleĀ  PubMedĀ  PubMed CentralĀ  Google ScholarĀ 

  • Slater PJB, Mann NI. Why do the females of many bird species sing in the tropics? J Avian Biol. 2004;35:289ā€“94.

    ArticleĀ  Google ScholarĀ 

  • Slender AL, Louter M, Gardner MG, Kleindorfer S. Thick-billed grasswren (Amytornis modestus) songs differ across subspecies and elicit different subspecific behavioural responses. Trans R Soc South Aust. 2018;142:105ā€“21.

    ArticleĀ  Google ScholarĀ 

  • Snijders L, van Oers K, Naguib M. Sex-specific responses to territorial intrusions in a communication network: evidence from radio-tagged great tits. Ecol Evol. 2017;7:918ā€“27.

    ArticleĀ  PubMedĀ  PubMed CentralĀ  Google ScholarĀ 

  • Toews DPL, Irwin DE. Cryptic speciation in a Holarctic passerine revealed by genetic and bioacoustic analyses. Mol Ecol. 2008;17:2691ā€“705.

    ArticleĀ  CASĀ  PubMedĀ  Google ScholarĀ 

  • TurčokovĆ” L, Osiejuk TS, Pavel V, Glapan J, PetruskovĆ” T. Song divergence of two Bluethroat subspecies (Luscinia s. svecica and L. s. cyanecula). Ornis Fenn. 2010;87:168ā€“79.

    Google ScholarĀ 

  • Valencia ID, Armenteras D. Habitat and distribution model of the Horned Lark (Eremophila alpestris peregrina) in the Altiplano of Cundinamarca and BoyacĆ”. Colombia Ornitol Colomb. 2004;2:25ā€“36.

    Google ScholarĀ 

  • Zuluaga-Bonilla JE, Macana-GarcĆ­a DC. Eremophila alpestris. In: Renjifo LM, Amaya-Villarreal AM, Burbano-GirĆ³n J, VelĆ”squez-TibatĆ” J, editors. Libr. rojo aves Colomb. Vol. II Ecosistemas abiertos, secos, Insul. acuĆ”ticos Cont. Mar. tierras altas del DariĆ©n y Sierra Nevada St. Marta y bosques hĆŗmedos del centro, norte y oriente del paĆ­s. 2nd ed. BogotĆ”-Colombia; 2016. p. 335ā€“9.

Download references

Acknowledgements

Special thanks to professor Pablo Emilio RodrĆ­guez Africano who managed the resources to develop this research and coordinated the data collection. To the ā€œCorporaciĆ³n AutĆ³noma Regional de BoyacĆ” (CORPOBOYACƁ)ā€ and the ā€œUniversidad PedagĆ³gica y TecnolĆ³gica de Colombia UPTCā€, for the financial and logistical support.

Funding

This work was supported by the ā€œCorporaciĆ³n AutĆ³noma Regional de BoyacĆ”ā€ and the ā€œUniversidad PedagĆ³gica y TecnolĆ³gica de Colombiaā€ under the research project CNV-025 of 2017, SGI-2292: ā€œHabitat preference and population density of the Horned lark (Eremophila alpestris peregrina) in La Copa Reservoir (Tocaā€“BoyacĆ”ā€“Colombia)ā€.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

JAV-P Took and processed the data, LAA-S wrote the manuscript and made the statistical analysis, and CR-M revised the manuscript. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Luis Alejandro Arias-Sosa.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

This work was done under a research project in collaboration with the ā€œCorporaciĆ³n AutĆ³noma Regional de BoyacĆ”ā€, the environmental entity that regulates wildlife management in the state. The experiments comply with the current Colombian laws regarding research in the biological field.

Consent for publication

Not applicable.

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Supplementary Information

Additional file 1: Table S1.

Records used in the song traits analysis. Table S2. PCA analysis of the songā€™s traits (using all the retrieved songs). Table S3. PCA analysis of the songā€™s traits (using one song per record). Table S4. PCA analysis of the play back response.

Additional file 2: Figure S1.

Representative spectrograms of two records (individuals) of the Nearctic and Neotropical Horned Larks.

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Vargas-Puentes, J.A., Arias-Sosa, L.A. & Ramos-MontaƱo, C. Song divergence indicates an unclear relationship between the Neotropical and Nearctic Horned Larks. Avian Res 12, 16 (2021). https://doi.org/10.1186/s40657-021-00251-y

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s40657-021-00251-y

Keywords