- Research
- Open access
- Published:
Egg recognition in Cinereous Tits (Parus cinereus): eggshell spots matter
Avian Research volume 10, Article number: 37 (2019)
Abstract
Background
Brood parasitic birds such as cuckoos (Cuculus spp.) can reduce their host’s reproductive success. Such selection pressure on the hosts has driven the evolution of defense behaviors such as egg rejection against cuckoo parasitism. Studies have shown that Cinereous Tits (Parus cinereus) in China have a good ability for recognizing foreign eggs. However, it is unclear whether egg spots play a role in egg recognition. The aims of our study were to investigate the egg recognition ability of two Cinereous Tit populations in China and to explore the role of spots in egg recognition.
Methods
To test the effect of eggshell spots on egg recognition, pure white eggs of the White-rumped Munia (Lonchura striata) and eggs of White-rumped Munia painted with red brown spots were used to simulate experimental parasitism.
Results
Egg experiments showed that Cinereous Tits rejected 51.5% of pure white eggs of the White-rumped Munia, but only 14.3% of spotted eggs of the White-rumped Munia. There was a significant difference in egg recognition and rejection rate between the two egg types.
Conclusions
We conclude that eggshell spots on Cinereous Tit eggs had a signaling function and may be essential to tits for recognizing and rejecting parasitic eggs.
Background
The mutual adaptations and counter-defense strategies between brood parasitic birds such as cuckoos (Cuculus spp.) and their hosts are comparable to an arms race for studying coevolution (Rothstein 1990; Davies 2000, 2015). Coevolutionary theory predicts that if brood parasitism is costly, hosts will evolve anti-parasite defense against brood parasitic birds, while brood parasitic birds will evolve parasitic strategies to crack their hosts’ defenses. To recognize and reject parasitic eggs is the most common and effective strategy among anti-brood parasite defense mechanisms (Davies 2000; Soler 2014), and the most common methods of hosts are therefore egg ejection and nest desertion (Šulc et al. 2019). In response to egg rejection by hosts, many parasitic birds evolve counter-adaptations to overcome the hosts’ defenses by laying mimicking (Brooke and Davies 1988; Avilés et al. 2006; Stoddard and Stevens 2010; Yang et al. 2010; Antonov et al. 2012; Abernathy et al. 2017; Meshcheryagina et al. 2018) or hidden eggs (Brooker and Brooker 1989; Brooker et al. 1990; Langmore et al. 2009; Gloag et al. 2014). Currently, more than 60% of cuckoo species lay eggs that mimic one or many different host eggs (Johnsgard 1997; Payne 2005). Generally, parasitic eggs mimic the background color, spot size, spot color, spot distribution, and shapes of the host eggs (Avilés et al. 2006; Starling et al. 2006; Spottiswoode and Stevens 2010; Antonov et al. 2012; de la Colina et al. 2012; Honza et al. 2014; Stoddard et al. 2014; Abernathy et al. 2017; Attard et al. 2017; Meshcheryagina et al. 2018). Egg mimicry makes it more challenging for hosts to detect the parasitic eggs (Cassey et al. 2008; Spottiswoode and Stevens 2010). And many hosts cannot recognize the parasitic egg, and are therefore forced to accept foreign eggs (Davies 2000; Langmore et al. 2003; Stokke et al. 2016).
Variable egg colors and spotting patterns are unique characteristics of birds (Hauber 2014). The spots are mainly composed of two pigments, protoporphyrin and biliverdin (Kennedy and Vevers 1976; Mikšík et al. 1996; Gorchein et al. 2009). There are many explanations for the role of the egg spots (Kilner 2006; Reynolds et al. 2009; Maurer et al. 2011; Stokke et al. 2017).
The first explanation is that spots are used as a protective coloration. Newton (1896) believed that spots can help to conceal the eggs and reduce the risk of predation (Kilner 2006; Hanley et al. 2013; Duval et al. 2016).
The second explanation is related to signal function for sexual selection. Some studies have shown that the pigments from the spots can imply the quality of the female or chick as well as the willingness of the female to reproduce, which affects male parental investment (Sanz and García-Navas 2009; López-de-Hierro and De Neve 2010; Stoddard and Stevens 2011; Stoddard et al. 2012; Hargitai et al. 2016; Poláček et al. 2017).
The third explanation is associated with structural function as egg spots can improve eggshell strength (Gosler et al. 2005; García-Navas et al. 2010; Bulla et al. 2012; Hargitai et al. 2013, 2016).
The fourth explanation is related to brood parasitism. Swynnerton (1918) proposed that the spots can be used in defense against cuckoo parasitism. This signature hypothesis predicts that cuckoo hosts should evolve less variation within their own clutches (to make it easier to pick out a parasitic egg), and more variation between the clutches of different females within a species (to make it harder for cuckoos evolve to a good match) (Swynnerton 1918). Much previous work agreed with this theoretical prediction (Davies and Brooke 1989a, b; Soler and Møller 1996; Stokke et al. 2002, 2007; Takasu 2003; Stoddard and Stevens 2010; Medina et al. 2016).
In fact, in response to cuckoo egg mimicry, hosts have evolved egg spotting patterns that can work as a unique “signature” for the female host (Davies 2000; Spottiswoode and Stevens 2010; Stoddard et al. 2014), which then makes it easier to detect foreign eggs (Swynnerton 1918; Stoddard and Stevens 2010; Davies 2011; Caves et al. 2015; Stokke et al. 2017). For example, the eggs of Tawny-flanked Prinia (Prinia subflava) have noticeable signatures, i.e., different egg color, egg spot size, egg shape, and distribution of egg spots, and they can accurately identify foreign eggs that are different from own eggs using these features (Spottiswoode and Stevens 2010). More recently, an experimental study showed that a frequent host of the parasitic Shiny Cowbird (Molothrus bonariensis), the Chalk-browed Mockingbirds (Mimus saturninus), rejected spotted eggs (32.4% of trials, n = 11 of 34 eggs) less than unspotted eggs (58.3% of trials, n = 21 of 36 eggs), irrespective of color (Hanley et al. 2019).
The Great Tit (Parus major) is a passerine bird in the tit family Paridae and is grouped together with numerous other subspecies. However, DNA studies have shown these other subspecies to be distinctive from the Great Tit, and have now been separated into two distinct species, the Cinereous Tit (Parus cinereus) of southern Asia, and the Japanese Tit (P. minor) of East Asia. The Great Tit remains the most widespread species in the genus Parus (Päckert et al. 2005). Recent studies have shown that coevolutionary interactions have existed between tits and cuckoos in China (Liang et al. 2016; Yang et al. 2019). In China, the Cinereous Tit has evolved the ability to recognize eggs and the rejection of non-mimetic eggs by Cinereous Tits is from 54.1% up to 100% (Liang et al. 2016). However, whether spots play a role in egg recognition has not been studied. The aims of our study were to further test the egg recognition ability of Cinereous Tits and to explore the role of eggshell spots in egg recognition in particular.
Methods
Study area
The study was carried out in Saihanba National Forest Park, Weichang, Hebei (42°02ʹ–42°36ʹN, 116°51ʹ–117°39ʹE). It is the main natural secondary forest and plantation forest area in Hebei, with an altitude of 1500 m. It has a cold temperate continental monsoon climate (Liu et al. 2017). Zuojia Nature Reserve was in Jilin, northeastern China (44°1ʹ–45°0ʹN, 126°0ʹ–126°8ʹE). The mean altitude is 300 m, with a continental monsoon climate and four distinct seasons in the temperate zone. Vegetation is temperate needle broad-leaved mixed forest zone with secondary forest (Yu et al. 2017). We monitored Cinereous Tits nesting in nest boxes during the breeding seasons each year. The nest boxes were attached to trees about 3 m above the ground, facing in a random direction (Liang et al. 2016; Yu et al. 2017).
Egg experiment
Field experiments were carried out from April to August in 2018 and 2019. The pure white eggs (without spots) of White-rumped Munias (Lonchura striata) show the same color as the background of Cinereous Tit eggs (Fig. 1a). The eggs of White-rumped Munias (1.28 ± 0.12 g in egg mass, 16.20 ± 0.82 mm × 12.46 ± 0.68 mm in egg size, n = 15) are a bit smaller than the eggs of Cinereous Tits (1.42 ± 0.20 g in egg mass, 17.03 ± 0.72 mm × 13.00 ± 0.36 mm in egg size, n = 136). Except this, the only difference between White-rumped Munias and Cinereous Tit eggs was that munias eggs were without spots (Fig. 1a). We used professional marker (APM 25201, M&G) to speckle the eggs of White-rumped Munias to make them more similar to those of tits (Fig. 1b).
We checked the nest boxes regularly to determine the breeding status of the tits. One or two days after the clutch was completed, we experimentally parasitized nests by (1) adding one pure white eggs of White-rumped Munias (without spots) to the nests of Cinereous Tits; or (2) by adding one speckled White-rumped Munia eggs (with spots) to the nests of Cinereous Tits. The experimental nests were re-examined after 5 days. On the 6th day, if the experimental egg was still in the nest, and the host had not deserted, the experimental egg was considered accepted by the host. However, if the experimental egg had disappeared or was damaged (but not own eggs), it was considered rejected by the host. If the nests were preyed on or destroyed within the 6 days, they were excluded from the experiment (Liang et al. 2016). We did four experiments at these two sites, only experiment with white eggs in Zuojia was carried out in 2018 and the rest of experiments in 2019.
Data analysis and statistics
Statistical analysis was performed using IBM SPSS 25.0 for Windows (IBM Inc., USA). Rejection rates of White-rumped Munia eggs and speckled White-rumped Munia eggs were compared using Fisher’s exact test. All tests were two-tailed, with statistical significance at p < 0.05. The data were expressed as mean ± standard deviation (mean ± SD).
Results
Egg recognition
In the Saihanba region in Hebei, the rejection rate of munia eggs and speckled munia eggs by Cinereous Tits was 50% (n = 18) and 15.4% (n = 26), respectively (Fig. 2). In the Zuojia Nature Reserve in Jilin, the rejection rate of munia eggs and speckled munia eggs by Cinereous Tits was 53.3% (n = 15) and 11.1% (n = 9) (Fig. 2). Cinereous tits from the two populations showed a high rejection of munia eggs and a low rejection of speckled munia eggs. And Cinereous Tits from the two populations showed no differences in their ability to recognize the two types of experimental eggs (Fisher’s exact test, all p > 0.05). Thus, these results were combined. Experimental parasitism results showed that the recognition rates of White-rumped Munia eggs and speckled White-rumped Munia eggs by Cinereous Tits were 51.5% (n = 33) and 14.3% (n = 35), respectively. There was a significant difference in the recognition rate between the two egg types (Fisher’s exact test, χ 2 = 10.757, df= 1, p = 0.002).
Discussion
From the eye of human being, the pure white eggs of White-rumped Munia appeared similar to Cinereous Tit eggs in the background color. But our results indicated that Cinereous Tits have high recognition ability for White-rumped Munia eggs. It could be due to the fact that from the Cinereous Tit’s point of view there is still a difference in the background color between the eggs of the White-rumped Munia and the Cinereous Tit’s own eggs. For example, tits could perceive UV color that humans cannot see (Cuthill et al. 2000) and therefore they could potentially reject experimental eggs based on these UV signals (Šulc et al. 2016). It could also be that the eggs of White-rumped Munia lack spots compared with those of Cinereous Tits themselves. However, when we speckled the eggs of White-rumped Munias, the rejection rate of speckled munia eggs by Cinereous Tits was significantly lower than that of the non-speckled ones, which suggests that eggshell spots play an important role in egg recognition for Cinereous Tits. Swynnerton (1918) proposed that eggshell spots act as signatures, and are used as a brood parasitism defense strategy, mainly against cuckoo egg mimicry. Our results show that eggshell spots may play a key role in identifying and rejecting parasitic eggs for Cinereous Tits. Numerous studies have been undertaken on the role of egg spots in identifying foreign eggs, and several perspectives from these studies are summarized as follows: (1) egg spots do not contribute to egg recognition, and egg size is the main clue to identifying eggs (Mason and Rothstein 1986; Marchetti 2000; Langmore et al. 2003); (2) spot patterns and egg colors are more important than egg size for egg recognition (Rothstein 1978; Lawes and Kirkman 1996; Igic et al. 2015; Antonov et al. 2006); (3) both egg colors and spot patterns affect egg rejection behavior by the host, but the egg colors are more important than the spot patterns (Lahti and Lahti 2002; Spottiswoode and Stevens 2010; Luro et al. 2018); (4) egg spot patterns are more important than egg colors for egg recognition (Underwood and Sealy 2006; López-De-Hierro and Moreno-Rueda 2009; de la Colina et al. 2012; Šulc et al. 2016); and (5) spots on the blunt pole of the egg provide a cue for egg recognition (Lahti and Lahti 2002; Polačiková et al. 2007, 2010; Polačiková and Grim 2010). There were so many different signals influencing the host’s response. As bird eggs themselves contain many characteristics, e.g., the background color, spot size, spot color, spot distribution, shapes and sizes of the host eggs, hosts may use the most available cues in making rejection decision, or may integrate several different egg characteristics in making such decision (Rothstein 1982; Spottiswoode and Stevens 2010). For example, some birds breed in the darkness of the tree cavities or deep burrows where visual cues are limited, tactile sensations become sensitive and thus egg size may play an important role in egg recognition (Mason and Rothstein 1986). However, in general, our experiment clearly shows that egg spots may play a role in the egg recognition process of Cinereous Tit. Although our study found that egg spots of Cinereous Tits may be essential for identifying and rejecting foreign eggs, it was unclear how the egg spots were deciphered by the tits and this required further investigation.
Conclusions
Our findings suggest that egg spots of Cinereous Tits are important clues for egg recognition. However, the specific characteristics of the spot patterns, such as spot distribution, spot size, spot brilliance, chroma, hue, and UV may also be potential clues for egg identification. We suggest that future studies should focus on which of these features determine egg recognition and egg rejection behavior in Cinereous Tits and other tit species.
Availability of data and materials
The datasets used and analyzed in this study are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.
References
Abernathy VE, Troscianko J, Langmore NE. Egg mimicry by the pacific koel: mimicry of one host facilitates exploitation of other hosts with similar egg types. J Avian Biol. 2017;48:1114–24.
Antonov A, Stokke BG, Moksnes A, Røskaft E. Egg rejection in marsh warblers (Acrocephalus palustris) heavily parasitized by common cuckoos (Cuculus canorus). Auk. 2006;123:419–30.
Antonov A, Stokke BG, Fossøy F, Ranke PS, Liang W, Yang C, et al. Are cuckoos maximizing egg mimicry by selecting host individuals with better matching egg phenotypes? PLoS ONE. 2012;7:e31704.
Attard MRG, Medina I, Langmore NE, Sherratt E. Egg shape mimicry in parasitic cuckoos. J Evol Biol. 2017;30:2079–84.
Avilés JM, Stokke BG, Moksnes A, Roskaft E, Åsmul M, Møller AP. Rapid increase in cuckoo egg matching in a recently parasitized reed warbler population. J Evol Biol. 2006;19:1901–10.
Brooker MG, Brooker LC. Cuckoo hosts in Australia. Austr Zool Rev. 1989;2:1–67.
Brooke ML, Davies NB. Egg mimicry by cuckoos Cuculus canorus in relation to discrimination by hosts. Nature. 1988;335:630–2.
Brooker LC, Brooker MG, Brooker AMH. An alternative population/genetics model for the evolution of egg mimesis and egg crypsis in cuckoos. J Theor Biol. 1990;146:123–43.
Bulla M, Šálek M, Gosler AG. Eggshell spotting does not predict male incubation but marks thinner areas of a shorebird’s shells. Auk. 2012;129:26–35.
Cassey P, Honza M, Grim T, Hauber ME. The modelling of avian visual perception predicts behavioural rejection responses to foreign egg colours. Biol Lett. 2008;4:515–7.
Caves EM, Stevens M, Iversen ES, Spottiswoode CN. Hosts of avian brood parasites have evolved egg signatures with elevated information content. Proc R Soc Lond B-Biol Sci. 2015;282:772–5.
Cuthill IC, Partridge JC, Bennett AT, Church SC, Hart NS, Hunt S. Ultraviolet vision in birds. Adv Study Behav. 2000;29:159–214.
Davies NB. Cuckoos, cowbirds and other cheats. London: T & AD Poyser; 2000.
Davies NB. Cuckoo adaptations: trickery and tuning. J Zool. 2011;284:1–14.
Davies NB. Cuckoo: cheating by nature. London: Bloomsbury; 2015.
Davies NB, Brooke ML. An experimental study of co-evolution between the cuckoo, Cuculus canorus, and its hosts. I. Host egg recognition. J Anim Ecol. 1989a;58:207–24.
Davies NB, Brooke ML. An experimental study of co-evolution between the cuckoo, Cuculus canorus, and its hosts. II. Host egg markings, chick discrimination and general discussion. J Anim Ecol. 1989b;58:225–36.
de la Colina MA, Pompilio L, Hauber ME, Reboreda JC, Mahler B. Different recognition cues reveal the decision rules used for egg rejection by hosts of a variably mimetic avian brood parasite. Anim Cogn. 2012;15:881–9.
Duval C, Cassey P, Lovell PG, Mikšík I, Reynolds SJ, Spencer KA. Maternal influence on eggshell maculation: implications for cryptic camouflaged eggs. J Ornithol. 2016;157:303–10.
García-Navas V, Sanz JJ, Merino S, Puente MDL, Lobato E, Cerro SD, et al. Experimental evidence for the role of calcium in eggshell pigmentation pattern and breeding performance in blue tits Cyanistes caeruleus. J Ornithol. 2010;152:71–82.
Gloag R, Keller LA, Langmore NE. Cryptic cuckoo eggs hide from competing cuckoos. Proc R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 2014;281:20141014.
Gorchein A, Lim CK, Cassey P. Extraction and analysis of colourful eggshell pigments using HPLC and HPLC/electrospray ionization tandem mass spectrometry. Biomed Chromatogr. 2009;23:602–6.
Gosler AG, Higham JP, Reynolds SJ. Why are birds’ eggs spotted? Ecol Lett. 2005;8:1105–13.
Hanley D, Cassey P, Doucet SM. Parents, predators, parasites, and the evolution of eggshell colour in open nesting birds. Evol Ecol. 2013;27:593–617.
Hanley D, López AV, Fiorini VD, Reboreda JC, Grim T, Hauber ME. Variation in multicomponent recognition cues alters egg rejection decisions: a test of the optimal acceptance threshold hypothesis. Philos Trans R Soc B. 2019;374:20180195.
Hargitai R, Nagy G, Herényi M, Török J. Effects of experimental calcium availability, egg parameters and laying order on great tit Parus major eggshell pigmentation patterns. Ibis. 2013;155:561–70.
Hargitai R, Heréhyi M, Nagy G, Nyiri Z, Eke Z, Török J. Effects of environmental conditions on the egg mass, yolk antioxidant level, eggshell thickness and eggshell spotting patterns of great tits (Parus major). J Ornithol. 2016;157:995–1006.
Hauber ME. The book of eggs: A life-size guide to the eggs of six hundred of the world’s bird species. Brighton: Ivy Press; 2014.
Honza M, Šulc M, Jelínek V, Požgayová M, Procházka P. Brood parasites lay eggs matching the appearance of host clutches. Proc Roy Soc B Biol Sci. 2014;281:20132665.
Igic B, Nunez V, Voss HU, Croston R, Aidala Z, López AV, et al. Using 3D printed eggs to examine the egg-rejection behaviour of wild birds. Peer J. 2015;3:e965.
Johnsgard PA. The avian brood parasites: deception at the nest. Oxford: Oxford University Press; 1997.
Kennedy GY, Vevers HG. A survey of avian eggshell pigments. Comp Biochem Physiol B Comp Biochem. 1976;55:117–23.
Kilner RM. The evolution of egg colour and patterning in birds. Biol Rev. 2006;81:383–406.
Lahti DC, Lahti AR. How precise is egg discrimination in weaverbirds? Anim Behav. 2002;63:1135–42.
Langmore NE, Hunt S, Kilner RM. Escalation of a coevolutionary arms race through host rejection of brood parasitic young. Nature. 2003;422:157–60.
Langmore NE, Stevens M, Maurer G, Kilner RM. Are dark cuckoo eggs cryptic in host nests? Anim Behav. 2009;78:461–8.
Lawes MJ, Kirkman S. Egg recognition and interspecific brood parasitism rates in red bishops (Aves: Ploceidae). Anim Behav. 1996;52:553–63.
Liang W, Møller AP, Stokke BG, Yang C, Kovařík P, Wang H, et al. Geographic variation in egg ejection rate by great tits across 2 continents. Behav Ecol. 2016;27:1405–12.
Liu J, Ma L, Zhang Z, Gu D, Wang J, Li J, et al. Maximum frequency of songs reflects body size among male dusky warblers Phylloscopus fuscatus (Passeriformes: Phylloscopidae). Eur Zool J. 2017;84:186–92.
López-De-Hierro MDG, DeNeve L. Pigment limitation and female reproductive characteristics influence eggshell spottiness and ground colour variation in the house sparrow (Passer domesticus). J Ornithol. 2010;151:833–40.
López-De-Hierro MDG, Moreno-Rueda G. Egg-spot pattern rather than egg colour affects conspecific egg rejection in the house sparrow (Passer domesticus). Behav Ecol Sociobiol. 2009;64:317–24.
Luro AB, Igic B, Croston R, López AV, Shawkey MD, Hauber ME. Which egg features predict egg rejection responses in American robins? Replicating Rothstein’s (1982) study. Ecol Evol. 2018;8:1673–9.
Marchetti K. Egg rejection in a passerine bird: size does matter. Anim Behav. 2000;59:877–83.
Mason P, Rothstein SI. Coevolution and avian brood parasitism: cowbird eggs show evolutionary response to host discrimination. Evolution. 1986;40:1207–14.
Maurer G, Portugal SJ, Cassey P. An embryo’s eye view of avian eggshell pigmentation. J Avian Biol. 2011;42:494–504.
Medina I, Troscianko J, Stevens M, Langmore NE. Brood parasitism is linked to egg pattern diversity within and among species of Australian passerines. Am Nat. 2016;187:351–62.
Meshcheryagina SG, Mashanova A, Bachurin GN, Mitiay IS, Golovatin MG. Host species determines egg size in Oriental cuckoo. J Zool. 2018;306:147–55.
Mikšík I, Holáň V, Deyl Z. Avian eggshell pigments and their variability. Comp Biochem Physiol B Biochem Mol Biol. 1996;113:607–12.
Newton AV. A dictionary of birds. London: A & C Black; 1896.
Päckert M, Martens J, Eck S, Nazarenko AA, Valchuk OP, Petri B, et al. The great tit (Parus major)—a misclassified ring species. Biol J Linn Soc. 2005;86:153–74.
Payne RB. The cuckoos. Oxford: Oxford University Press; 2005.
Poláček M, Griggio M, Mikšík I, Bartíková M, Eckenfellner M, Hoi H. Eggshell coloration and its importance in postmating sexual selection. Ecol Evol. 2017;7:941–9.
Polačiková L, Grim T. Blunt egg pole holds cues for foreign egg discrimination: experimental evidence. J Avian Biol. 2010;41:111–6.
Polačiková L, Honza M, Procházka P, Topercer J, Stokke BG. Colour characteristics of the blunt egg pole: cues for recognition of parasitic eggs as revealed by reflectance spectrophotometry. Anim Behav. 2007;74:419–27.
Polačiková L, Stokke BG, Procházka P, Honza M, Moksnes A, Røskaft E. The role of blunt egg pole characteristics for recognition of eggs in the song thrush (Turdus philomelos). Behaviour. 2010;147:465–78.
Reynolds SJ, Martin GR, Cassey P. Is sexual selection blurring the functional significance of eggshell colouration hypotheses? Anim Behav. 2009;78:209–15.
Rothstein SI. Mechanisms of avian egg-recognition—additional evidence for learned components. Anim Behav. 1978;26:671–6.
Rothstein SI. Mechanisms of avian egg recognition: which egg parameters elicit responses by rejecter species? Behav Ecol Sociobiol. 1982;11:229–39.
Rothstein SI. A model system for coevolution: avian brood parasitism. Annu Rev Ecol Syst. 1990;21:481–508.
Sanz JJ, García-Navas V. Eggshell pigmentation pattern in relation to breeding performance of blue tits Cyanistes caeruleus. J Anim Ecol. 2009;78:31–41.
Soler M. Long-term coevolution between avian brood parasites and their hosts. Biol Rev. 2014;89:688–704.
Soler JJ, Møller AP. A comparative analysis of the evolution of variation in appearance of eggs of European passerines in relation to brood parasitism. Behav Ecol. 1996;7:89–94.
Spottiswoode CN, Stevens M. Visual modeling shows that avian host parents use multiple visual cues in rejecting parasitic eggs. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2010;107:8672–6.
Starling M, Heinsohn R, Cockburn A, Langmore NE. Cryptic gentes revealed in pallid cuckoos Cuculus pallidus using reflectance spectrophotometry. Proc R Soc B Biol Sci. 2006;273:1929–34.
Stoddard MC, Stevens M. Pattern mimicry of host eggs by the common cuckoo, as seen through a bird’s eye. Proc R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 2010;277:1387–93.
Stoddard MC, Stevens M. Avian vision and the evolution of egg color mimicry in the common cuckoo. Evolution. 2011;65:2004–13.
Stoddard MC, Fayet A, Kilner RM, Hinde C. Egg spots patterns do not advertise offspring quality or influence male provisioning in great tits. PLoS ONE. 2012;7:e40211.
Stoddard MC, Kilner RM, Town C. Pattern recognition algorithm reveals how birds evolve individual egg pattern signatures. Nat Commun. 2014;5:4117.
Stokke BG, Moksnes A, Røskaft E. Obligate brood parasites as selective agents for evolution of egg appearance in passerine birds. Evolution. 2002;56:199–205.
Stokke BG, Takasu F, Moksnes A, Røskaft E. The importance of clutch characteristics and learning for anti-parasite adaptations in hosts of avian brood parasites. Evolution. 2007;61:2212–28.
Stokke BG, Røskaft E, Moksnes A, Møller AP, Antonov A, Fossøy F, et al. Disappearance of eggs from nonparasitized nests of brood parasite hosts: the evolutionary equilibrium hypothesis revisited. Biol J Linn Soc. 2016;118:215–25.
Stokke BG, Fossøy F, Røskaft E, Moksnes A. Adaptations of brood parasitic eggs. In: Soler M, editor. Avian brood parasitism: behaviour, ecology, evolution and coevolution. Cham: Springer International Publishing AG; 2017. p. 363–84.
Šulc M, Procházka P, Capek M, Honza M. Birds use eggshell UV reflectance when recognizing non-mimetic parasitic eggs. Behav Ecol. 2016;27:677–84.
Šulc M, Troscianko J, Štětková G, Hughes AE, Jelínek V, Capek M, Honza M. Mimicry cannot explain rejection type in a host–brood parasite system. Anim Behav. 2019;155:111–8.
Swynnerton CFM. Rejections by birds of eggs unlike their own: with remarks on some of the cuckoo problems. Ibis. 1918;6:127–54.
Takasu F. Co-evolutionary dynamics of egg appearance in avian brood parasitism. Evol Ecol Res. 2003;5:345–62.
Underwood TJ, Sealy SG. Parameters of brown headed cowbird Molothrus ater egg discrimination in warbling vireos Vireo gilvus. J Avian Biol. 2006;37:457–66.
Yang C, Liang W, Cai Y, Shi S, Takasu F, Møller AP, et al. Coevolution in action: disruptive selection on egg colour in an avian brood parasite and its host. PLoS ONE. 2010;5:e10816.
Yang C, Liang W, Møller AP. Egg retrieval versus egg rejection in cuckoo hosts. Philos Trans R Soc B. 2019;374:20180200.
Yu J, Lv W, Xu H, Bibi N, Yu Y, Jiang Y, et al. Function of note strings in Japanese Tit alarm calls to the common cuckoo: a playback experiment. Avian Res. 2017;8:22.
Acknowledgements
We thank Saihanba National Forest Park of Hebei and Zuojia Nature Reserve of Jilin for their help and cooperation, and Jianwei Zhang, Xintong Li, Jing Yue and Hailin Lu for assistance with field work. We are also grateful to the two anonymous referees for their constructive comments.
Funding
This work was funded by the National Natural Science Foundation of China (Nos. 31772453 and 31970427 to WL, No. 31672303 to CY and No. 31770419 to HW).
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Contributions
WL designed the study. JL, JY and HW carried out field experiments. CY performed statistical analyses. JL wrote the draft manuscript, and WL revised and improved the manuscript. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.
Corresponding author
Ethics declarations
Ethics approval and consent to participate
The experiments reported here comply with the current laws of China. Fieldwork was carried out under the permission from Saihanba National Forest Park, Hebei, and Zuojia Nature Reserve, Jilin, China. Experimental procedures were in agreement with the Animal Research Ethics Committee of Hainan Provincial Education Centre for Ecology and Environment, Hainan Normal University (Permit No. HNECEE-2011-001).
Consent for publication
Not applicable.
Competing interests
The authors declare that they have no competing interests.
Rights and permissions
Open Access This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated.
About this article
Cite this article
Liu, J., Yang, C., Yu, J. et al. Egg recognition in Cinereous Tits (Parus cinereus): eggshell spots matter. Avian Res 10, 37 (2019). https://doi.org/10.1186/s40657-019-0178-1
Received:
Accepted:
Published:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s40657-019-0178-1